Recently, I was asked: “What made you interested in research
methods?” I’m afraid I didn’t give a good answer, but instead started
complaining about my eight-times
failure to replicate that nobody wants to publish. I have been thinking
about this question some more, and realised that my interest in research
methods and good science is driven by predominantly selfish reasons. This gave
me the idea to write a blog post: I think it is important to realise that
striving towards good science is, in the long run, beneficial to a researcher.
So let’s ignore the “how” for the time being (there are already many articles
and blog posts on this issue; see, for example, entries for an essay
contest by The Winnower) – let’s focus on the “why”.
The world as it
should be
Let’s imagine the research world
as it should (or could) be. Presumably, we all went into research because we
wanted to learn more about the world – and we wanted to actively contribute to
discovering new knowledge. Imagine that we live in a world where we can trust the
existing literature. Theories are based on experiments that are sound and
replicable. The job of a researcher is to keep up to date on this literature,
find gaps, and design experiments that can fill these gaps, thus providing a
more complete picture of the phenomenon they are studying.
The world as it is
The research world as it is
provides two sources of frustrations (at least, for me): (1) Playing Russian
Roulette when it comes to conducting experiments, and (2) sifting through a
literature which consists of an unknown ratio of manure to pearls, and trying
to find the pearls.
Russian Roulette
I have conducted numerous experiments during my PhD and
post-doc so far, and a majority of them “didn’t work”. By “didn’t work”, I mean
they showed non-significant p-values
when I expected an effect, showed different results from published experiments
(again, my eight-times
failure to replicate), and occasionally, they were just not designed very
well and I would get floor/ceiling effects. I attributed this to my own lack of
experience and competence. I looked to my colleagues had many published
experiments, and considered alternative career paths. In the last year of my
PhD, I came to a realisation: even
professors have the same problem.
In the research world as it is, a
researcher may come up with an idea for an experiment. It can be a great idea,
based on a careful evaluation of theories and models. The experiment can be
well-designed and neat, providing a pertinent test of the researcher’s
hypothesis. Then the data is collected and analysed – and it is discovered that
the experiment “didn’t work”. Shoulders are shrugged – the researcher moves on.
Occasionally, one experiment will “work” and can be published.
How is it possible, I asked
myself, that so much good research goes to waste, just because an experiment
“didn’t work”? Is it really necessary to completely discard a promising
question or theory, just because a first attempt at getting an answer “didn’t
work”? How many labs conduct experiments that “don’t work”, not knowing that
other labs have already tried and failed with the same approach? These are, as
of now, rhetorical questions, but I firmly believe that learning more about
research methods and how these can be used to produce sound and efficient
experiments can answer them.
Sifting through manure
Some theories are intuitively appealing, apparently elegant,
and elicit a lot of enthusiasm with a lot of people. New PhD students want to
“do something with this theory”, and try to do follow-up studies, only to find
that their follow-up experiments “don’t work”, replications of the experiments
that support the theory “don’t work”, and the theory doesn’t even make sense
when you really think about it. *
Scientists stand on the shoulders
of giants. Science cannot be done without relying on existing knowledge at
least to some extent. In an ideal world, our experiments and theories should
build on previous work. However, I often get the feeling that I am building on
manure instead of a sound foundation.
So, in order to try and
understand whether I can trust an effect, I sift through the papers on it. I
look for evidence of publication bias, dodgy-sounding post-hoc moderators or
trimming decisions, statistical and logical errors (such as concluding that the
difference between two groups is significant because one is significantly above
chance while the other is not); check whether studies with larger sample sizes
tend to give negative results, while positive results are predominantly
supported by studies with small samples. It’s a thankless job. I criticise and
question the work of colleagues, who are often in senior positions and may well
one day make decisions that affect my livelihood.
At the same time, I lack the time to conduct experiments to test and develop my
own ideas. But what else should I do? Close my eyes to these issues and just
work on my own line of research? Spending less or no time scrutinising the
existing literature would mean that I don’t know whether I am building
my research agenda on pearls or manure. This would mean that I could waste
months or years on a question that I should have known to be a dead end from
the very beginning.
Conclusion
So, why am I interested in research methods? Because it will
make research more efficient, for me personally. It is difficult to conduct a
good study, but in the long run, it should be no more difficult than running a
number of crappy studies and publishing the one that “worked”. It should also be
much less frustrating, much more rewarding, and in the end, we will do what we
(presumably) love: contribute to discovering new knowledge about how the world
works.
-----------------------------------------------------------------
* This example is fictional. Any resemblance to real persons
or events is purely coincidental.
A good research method is the first step to establish a theory. I really like your blog!!!
ReplyDeleteHelpful post.
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
ReplyDelete